Introduction
Schizophrenia is a severe, chronic and socially disabling mental disorder. Patients with schizophrenia suffer from delusions, hallucinations and multiple cognitive deficits. Working memory (WM) deficit is considered a core feature and cognitive biomarker in patients with schizophrenia.1–6
WM, a cognitive system that is responsible for temporarily holding information available for processing, is important for reasoning, decision-making and social behaviour.7 WM has been considered the central foundation of human high-level cognitive activity. It enables the interactions of multiple information from memory, sensory experience and motor control into process-oriented behaviour.8 The disturbance of this system can result in dysfunction of cognitive organisation, failure of self-monitoring, distractibility and other phenotypical features of schizophrenia.
WM involves three phases: novel information encoding; maintenance, updating and manipulation; and information retrieval.9–12 Researchers have demonstrated experimentally that neurons in the prefrontal cortex generate persistent firing to retain information.13 14 A number of studies have documented WM deficits in patients with schizophrenia. Zhang et al examined factors that exhibited influence on visual WM and demonstrated relationships between visual WM and multiple hospitalisations and the suffering caused by severe negative symptoms in patients with adult-onset schizophrenia.15 Gan et al found that patients with schizophrenia showed poor performance in the n-back task than patients with methamphetamine-associated psychosis.16 Studies on specific aspects of WM may provide pivotal evidence for the aetiology of a disease.
Functional MRI (fMRI) studies also provided detailed evidence that the superior part of the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC) is likely involved in encoding of visual WM information17 18; the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) is crucial for maintenance19–21 and the inferior portion of the VLPFC is particularly important for retrieval.22 23 fMRI studies have suggested that visuospatial WM impairment is associated with reduced prefrontal neuronal activation.24–26 Schlösser et al reported a reduced activation in the anterior cingulate cortex and the DLPFC in patients with schizophrenia during the encoding phase of WM.27 The meta-analysis of fMRI studies has consistently documented an abnormal activation in the DLPFC during WM in patients with schizophrenia.27–29 Additionally, positron emission tomography has revealed a functional disconnection in prefrontal–parietal networks during WM processing in these patients.30
Their findings have confirmed that WM is a robust feature of schizophrenia across different paradigms, especially in visuospatial WM tasks.25 26 However, whether the visual WM deficit extends to non-spatial domains remains unknown. Several studies have reported prominent object WM deficits in patients with schizophrenia, suggesting that visual WM in these patients extends to non-spatial domains.31–33
This study primarily aimed to identify the processing of visual object WM, including house and face WM, processing under different condition levels of memory load in patients with first-episode schizophrenia. Studies of non-spatial WM could greatly advance our understanding of the capacity of visual WM in patients with schizophrenia. Further, the two visual tasks we applied could be used to distinguish different types of visual stimuli, especially in recognition of face specificity.32 34 Additionally, this study was designed to explore the relation patterns between WM capacity, including clinical symptoms and other functions, during visual object WM processing in patients with schizophrenia in comparison with healthy control participants. We hypothesised that patients with schizophrenia would show a reduced visual object WM processing and association with cognitive performances.